“Deadeye: A Novel Preattentive Visualization Technique Based on Dichoptic Presentation” by Krekhov, Krüger and Cmentowski
Conference:
Type(s):
Interest Area:
- Research / Education
Title:
- Deadeye: A Novel Preattentive Visualization Technique Based on Dichoptic Presentation
Session/Category Title: IEEE TVCG Session on Advances in Data Visualization
Presenter(s)/Author(s):
Abstract:
Preattentive visual features such as hue or flickering can effectively draw attention to an object of interest — for instance, an important feature in a scientific visualization. These features appear to pop out and can be recognized by our visual system, independently from the number of distractors. Most cues do not take advantage of the fact that most humans have two eyes. In cases where binocular vision is applied, it is almost exclusively used to convey depth by exposing stereo pairs. We present Deadeye, a novel preattentive visualization technique based on presenting different stimuli to each eye. The target object is rendered for one eye only and is instantly detected by our visual system. In contrast to existing cues, Deadeye does not modify any visual properties of the target and, thus, is particularly suited for visualization applications. Our evaluation confirms that Deadeye is indeed perceived preattentively. We also explore a conjunction search based on our technique and show that, in contrast to 3D depth, the task cannot be processed in parallel.
References:
[1] D. Alais and R. Blake. Binocular rivalry. MIT press, 2005.
[2] B. Alper, T. Hollerer, J. Kuchera-Morin, and A. Forbes. Stereoscopic highlighting: 2d graph visualization on stereo displays. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 17(12):2325?2333, Dec. 2011. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2011.234
[3] S. Anstis and A. Ho. Nonlinear combination of luminance excursions during flicker, simultaneous contrast, afterimages and binocular fusion. Vision Research, 38(4):523?539, 1998.
[4] D. H. Baker. Decoding eye-of-origin outside of awareness. NeuroImage, 147:89?96, 2017.
[5] B. Bauer, P. Jolicoeur, and W. B. Cowan. Visual search for targets that are or are not linearly separable from distracters. 36:1439?65, 06 1996.
[6] R. Blake. A neural theory of binocular rivalry. Psychological review, 96(1):145, 1989.
[7] A. Borji and L. Itti. State-of-the-art in visual attention modeling. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 35(1):185207, Jan 2013. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2012.89
[8] B. Caziot and B. T. Backus. Stereoscopic offset makes objects easier to recognize. PLoS ONE, 10(6):e0129101, 2015. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone. 0129101
[9] C.-Y. Cheng, M.-Y. Yen, H.-Y. Lin, W.-W. Hsia, and W.-M. Hsu. Association of ocular dominance and anisometropic myopia. Investigative ophthalmology & visual science, 45(8):2856?2860, 2004.
[10] F. Cole, D. DeCarlo, A. Finkelstein, K. Kin, K. Morley, and A. Santella. Directing gaze in 3d models with stylized focus. In Proceedings of the 17th Eurographics Conference on Rendering Techniques, EGSR ?06, pp. 377387. Eurographics Association, Aire-la-Ville, Switzerland, Switzerland, 2006. doi: 10.2312/EGWR/EGSR06/377-387
[11] C. M. De Weert and W. J. M. Levelt. Binocular brightness combinations: Additive and nonadditive aspects. Perception & Psychophysics, 15(3):551562, 1974.
[12] J. T. Enns and R. A. Rensink. Influence of scene-based properties on visual search. Science, 247(4943):721, 1990.
[13] J. T. Enns and R. A. Rensink. Sensitivity to three-dimensional orientation in visual search. Psychological Science, 1(5):323?326, 1990.
[14] M. A. Formankiewicz and J. Mollon. The psychophysics of detecting binocular discrepancies of luminance. Vision research, 49(15):1929?1938, 2009.
[15] J. Friedenberg. Visual attention and consciousness. Psychology Press, 2012.
[16] C. Gutwin, A. Cockburn, and A. Coveney. Peripheral popout: The inf luence of visual angle and stimulus intensity on popout effects. In Proceedings of the 2017 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI ?17, pp. 208?219. ACM, New York, NY, USA, 2017. doi: 10.1145/3025453.3025984
[17] K. Hall, C. Perin, P. Kusalik, and C. Gutwin. Formalizing emphasis in information visualization. In Computer Graphics Forum, vol. 35, pp. 717?737, 2016.
[18] J. M. Harris and L. M. Wilcox. The role of monocularly visible regions in depth and surface perception. Vision research, 49(22):2666?2685, 2009.
[19] S. G. Hart and L. E. Staveland. Development of nasa-tlx (task load index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. Advances in psychology, 52:139?183, 1988.
[20] C. Healey and J. Enns. Attention and visual memory in visualization and computer graphics. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 18(7):1170?1188, July 2012. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2011.127
[21] R. Hoffmann, P. Baudisch, and D. S. Weld. Evaluating visual cues for window switching on large screens. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI ?08, pp. 929?938. ACM, NewYork, NY, USA, 2008. doi: 10.1145/1357054.1357199
[22] L. Huang and H. Pashler. A boolean map theory of visual attention. Psychological review, 114(3):599, 2007.
[23] D. E. Huber and C. G. Healey. Visualizing data with motion. In Visualization, 2005. VIS 05. IEEE, pp. 527?534. IEEE, 2005.
[24] L. Itti and C. Koch. Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 2(3):194, 2001.
[25] B. Julesz. Binocular depth perception of computer-generated patterns. Bell Labs Technical Journal, 39(5):1125?1162, 1960.
[26] B. Julesz. Foundations of cyclopean perception. 1971.
[27] B. Julesz et al. Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions. Nature, 290(5802):91?97, 1981.
[28] N. K. Logothetis, D. A. Leopold, and D. L. Sheinberg. What is rivalling during binocular rivalry? Nature, 380(6575):621, 1996.
[29] C. Malamed. Visual language for designers: principles for creating graphics that people understand. Rockport Publishers, 2009.
[30] D. Marr and T. Poggio. A computational theory of human stereo vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 204(1156):301?328, 1979.
[31] D. Marr, T. Poggio, et al. Cooperative computation of stereo disparity. From the Retina to the Neocortex, pp. 239?243, 1976.
[32] P. McLeod, J. Driver, and J. Crisp. Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel. Nature, 332(6160):154?155, 1988.
[33] H. Mochizuki, N. Shoji, E. Ando, M. Otsuka, K. Takahashi, and T. Handa. The magnitude of stereopsis in peripheral visual fields. Kitasato Med J, 41:1?5, 2012.
[34] H. J. M?uller and A. von Muhlenen. Visual search for conjunctions of motion and form: The efficiency of attention to static versus moving items depends on practice. Visual Cognition, 6(3-4):385?408, 1999.
[35] A. L. Nagy and R. R. Sanchez. Critical color differences determined with a visual search task. JOSA A, 7(7):1209?1217, 1990.
[36] K. Nakayama and S. Shimojo. Da vinci stereopsis: Depth and subjective occluding contours from unpaired image points. Vision Research, 30(11):1811? 1825, 1990. Optics Physiology and Vision. doi: 10.1016/ 0042-6989(90)90161-D
[37] K.NakayamaandG.H.Silverman. Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions. Nature, 320(6059):264?265, 1986.
[38] D. Noton and L. Stark. Scanpaths in saccadic eye movements while viewing and recognizing patterns. Vision Research, 11(9):929? IN8, 1971. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(71)90213-6
[39] A. J. O?Toole and C. L. Walker. On the preattentive accessibility of stereoscopic disparity: Evidence from visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(2):202?218, 1997.
[40] C. L. Paffen, R. S. Hessels, and S. Van der Stigchel. Interocular conflict attracts attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(2):251?256, 2012.
[41] C. L. E. Paffen, I. T. C. Hooge, J. S. Benjamins, and H. Hogendoorn. A search asymmetry for interocular conflict. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(4):1042?1053, May 2011. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0100-3
[42] C. Porac and S. Coren. The dominant eye. Psychological bulletin, 83(5):880, 1976.
[43] J. Schild. Deep gaming : the creative and technological potential of stereoscopic 3D vision for interactive entertainment. PhD thesis, [Charleston, SC], 2014.
[44] J. Schild and M. Masuch. Formalizing the potential of stereoscopic 3d user experience in interactive entertainment. vol. 9391, pp. 93911D?93911D12, 2015.
[45] B. Suh, A. Woodruff, R. Rosenholtz, and A. Glass. Popout prism: Adding perceptual principles to overview+detail document interfaces. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI ?02, pp. 251?258. ACM, New York, NY, USA, 2002. doi: 10.1145/503376.503422
[46] D. Y. Teller and E. Galanter. Brightnesses, luminances, and fechner?s paradox. Perception & Psychophysics, 2(7):297?300, 1967.
[47] J. T. Townsend. Serial vs. parallel processing: Sometimes they look like tweedledum and tweedledee but they can (and should) be distinguished. Psychological Science, 1(1):46?54, 1990.
[48] A. Treisman and S. Gormican. Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological review, 95(1):15, 1988.
[49] A. Treisman and J. Souther. Illusory words: The roles of attention and of top?down constraints in conjoining letters to form words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(1):3, 1986.
[50] A. M. Treisman and G. Gelade. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive psychology, 12(1):97?136, 1980.
[51] I. Tsirlin, L. M. Wilcox, and R. S. Allison. Da vinci decoded: Does da vinci stereopsis rely on disparity? Journal of Vision, 12(12):2?2, 2012.
[52] M. Waldner, M. L. Muzic, M. Bernhard, W. Purgathofer, and I. Viola. Attractive flicker: Guiding attention in dynamic narrative visualizations. IEEETransactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 20(12):2456
[53] C. Ware. Information visualization: perception for design. Elsevier, 2012.
[54] J. M. Wolfe, K. R. Cave, and S. L. Franzel. Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 15(3):419, 1989.
[55] J. M. Wolfe and S. L. Franzel. Binocularity and visual search. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 44(1):81?93, 1988.
[56] J. M. Wolfe and T. S. Horowitz. Five factors that guide attention in visual search. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(3):0058, 2017.
[57] A. Yarbus. Eye movements and vision. Plenum Press, New York, 1967.
[58] A. L. Yarbus. Eye movements during perception of complex objects. In Eye movements and vision, pp. 171?211. Springer, 1967.
[59] H. Zhang. Spectacularly Binocular: Exploiting Binocular Luster Effects for HCI Applications. PhD thesis, School of Computing, Natuanal University of Singapore, 2014.
[60] H. Zhang, X. Cao, and S. Zhao. Beyond stereo: An exploration of unconventional binocular presentation for novel visual experience. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI ?12, pp. 2523?2526. ACM, New York, NY, USA, 2012. doi: 10.1145/2207676.2208638
[61] L. Zhaoping. Attention capture by eye of origin singletons even without awareness?a hallmark of a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 8(5):1?1, 2008.
[62] B. Zou, I. S. Utochkin, Y. Liu, and J. M. Wolfe. Binocularity and visual search?revisited. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(2):473?483, 2017.